ALPHABETICAL BRAIN® VOCABULARY
HUMANIST GALAXY
OF SECULAR SCIENCE STARS
ANTONIO DAMASIO

June 5, 2020

green separator

SELF COMES TO MIND:
Constructing the Conscious Brain
by Antonio R. Damasio
Pantheon Books, 2010
(367 pages)

green separator
    Quote = "Antonio Damasio introduces an evolutionary perspective that entails a radical change in the way the story of the history of conscious minds is viewed and told. He also advances a radical hypothesis regarding the origins and varieties of feelings, which is central to his framework for the 'biological construction of consciousness.' Feelings are grounded in the apparent fusion of body and brain networks. Feelings first emerge from the historically old and rote memory of the brainstem rather than from the modern executive functions of the cerebral cortex." (Paraphrased by webmaster from the publisher's blurb)

    Quote = "Damasio suggests that the brain's development of a 'human self' becomes a challenge to nature's indifference and opens the way for the appearance of culture. This is a radical break in the course of evolution and the source of a new level of life regulation that he calls 'socio-cultural homeostasis.' This book is a groundbreaking journey into the neurobiological foundations of the mind and the self, which is based on the genetically well-established medical concept about the body's homeostasis or balance." (Paraphrased by webmaster from the publisher's blurb)

    Quote = "As he has done previously, USC neuroscientist Antonio Damasio explores the process which leads to human consciousness. And as he also has done previously, he alternates between some exquisite passages which represent the best that popular science has concocted to explain some of the technical verbiage that few will be able to follow. In addition, he draws meaningful distinctions among points on the continuum from brain to mind ('from consciousness to self')." (Paraphrased by webmaster from the publisher's blurb)

    "Damasio constantly attempts to understand the evolutionary reasons why each arose and attempts to tie each to an underlying physical reality. He goes to great lengths to explain that many species, such as social insects, have 'minds.' However, humans are distinguished by their 'autobiographical self,' which adds flexibility and creativity, and has led to the development of culture. Culture is a 'radical novelty' in natural history." (Paraphrased by webmaster from the Publisher's Weekly Review)
green separator
BOOK OUTLINE
green separator

PART 1 — STARTING OVER (1-60)

1) AWAKENING (3-30)
    [1] Goals and reasons ()

    [2] Approaching the problem ()

    [3] The self as witness ()

    [4] Overcoming a misleading intuition ()

    [5] An integrated perspective ()

    [6] The framework ()

    [7] A preview of main ideas ()

    [8] Life and the conscious mind ()
2) FROM LIFE REGULATION TO BIOLOGICAL VALUE (31-60)
    [1] The implausibility of reality ()

    [2] Natural will ()

    [3] Staying alive ()

    [4] The origins of homeostasis ()

    [5] Cells, multicellular organisms, and engineered machines ()

    [6] Biological value ()

    [7] Biological value in whole organisms ()

    [8] The success of our early forerunners ()

    [9] Developing incentives (52-55)

    [10] Connecting homeostasis, value, and consciousness (55-60)
PART 2 — WHAT'S IN A BRAIN THAT A MIND CAN BE? (61-)

3) MAKING MAPS AND MAKING IMAGES (63-88)
    [1] Maps and images ()

    [2] Cutting below the surface ()

    [3] Maps and minds ()

    [4] The neurology of mind ()

    [5] The beginnings of mind ()

    [6] Closer to the making of mind? (86-88)
4) THE BODY IN MIND (89-
    [1] The topic of the mind ()

    [2] Body mapping ()

    [3] From body to brain (96-97)

    [4] Representing quantities and constructing qualities (97-100)

    [5] Primordial feelings (101)

    [6] Mapping body states and simulating body states (101-104)

    [7] The source of an idea (104-106)

    [8] The body-minded brain (106-107)
5) EMOTIONS AND FEELINGS (108-129)
    [1] Situating emotion and feeling ()

    [2] Defining emotion and feeling ()

    [3] Triggering and executing emotions ()

    [4] The strange case of william james ()

    [5] Feelings of emotion ()

    [6] How do we feel an emotion? ()

    [7] The timing of emotions and feelings ()

    [8] The varieties of emotion ()

    [9] Up and down the emotional range ()

    [10] An aside on admiration and compassion (-129)
6) AN ARCHITECTURE FOR MEMORY (130-153)
    [1] Somehow, somewhere (130-132)

    [2] The nature of memory records (132-133)

    [3] Dispositions came first, maps followed (133-136)

    [4] Memory at work (136-138)

    [5] A brief aside on kinds of memory (139-140)

    [6] A possible solution to the problem (140-141)

    [7] More on convergence-divergence zones (144-148)

    [8] The model at work (148-151)

    [9] The how and where of perception and recall (151-153)
PART 3 — BEING CONSCIOUS (155-)

7) CONSCIOUSNESS OBSERVED (157-179)
    [1] Defining consciousness ()

    [2] Breaking consciousness apart ()

    [3] Removing the self and keeping a mind ()

    [4] Completing a working definition ()

    [5] Kinds of consciousness ()

    [6] Human and nonhuman consciousness ()

    [7] What consciousness is not (-176)

    [8] The Freudian unconscious (177-179)
8) BUILDING A CONSCIOUS MIND (180-209)
    [1] A working hypothesis ()

    [2] Approaching the conscious brain ()

    [3] Previewing the conscious mind ()

    [4] The ingredients of a conscious mind ()

    [5] The proto-self ()

    [6] Constructing the core self ()

    [7] The core self state (-209)

    [8] Touring the brain as it constructs a conscious mind (208-209)
9) THE AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL SELF (210-240)
    [1] Memory made conscious (210-211)

    [2] Constructing the autobiographical self (212-213)

    [3] The issue of coordination (214)

    [4] The coordinators (215-217)

    [5] A possible role for the posteromedial cortices or PMCS (218-222)

    [6] The PMCS at work (222-224)

    [7] Other considerations on the posteromedial cortices (225-236)

    [8] A closing note on the pathologies of consciousness (236-240)
10) PUTTING IT TOGETHER (241-263)
    [1] By way of summary (241-243)

    [2] The neurology of consciousness (243-249)

    [3] The anatomical bottleneck behind the conscious mind (249-251)

    [4] From the ensemble work of large anatomical divisions to the work of neurons (251-253)

    [5] When we feel our perceptions (253-254)

    [6] Qualia I (254-256)

    [7] Qualia 2 (256)

    [8] Qualia and self (256-262)

    [9] Unfinished business (262-263)
PART 4 --- LONG AFTER CONSCIOUSNESS (265-297)

11) LIVING WITH CONSCIOUSNESS (267-297)
    [1] Why consciousness prevailed (267-268)

    [2] Self and the issue of control (269-272)

    [3] An aside on the unconscious (269-272)

    [4] A note on the genomic unconscious (273-278)

    [5] The feeling of conscious will (278-279)

    [6] Educating the cognitive unconscious (280-282)

    [7] Brain and justice (282-284)

    [8] Nature and culture (284-288)

    [9] Self comes to mind (288-290)

    [10] The consequences of a reflective self (290-297)
APPENDIX (299-317)
    [1] Brain Architecture (299-301)

    [2] Bricks and Mortar (301-306)

    [3] More on Large-Scale Architecture (306-310)

    [4] The Importance of Location (310-312)

    [5] At the Interfaces between the Brain and the World (312-314)

    [6] A Note on the Mind-Brain Equivalence Hypothesis (314-317)
NOTES (319-342)

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS (343-344)

INDEX (345-367)

green separator
AUTHOR NOTES, SUMMARY,
AND BOOK DESCRIPTION

green separator

AUTHOR NOTES = Antonio Damasio was born in Lisbon, Portugal and studied medicine at the University of Lisbon Medical School, where he also did his neurological residency and completed his doctorate. Eventually, he moved to the United States as a research fellow at the Aphasia Research Center in Boston. From 1976 to 2005, he was M.W. Van Allen Professor and Head of Neurology at the University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics. He is currently the David Dornsife Professor of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Neurology, and director of the Brain and Creativity Institute at the University of Southern California.

Damasio has written several books on his research including Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain, which won the Science et Vie prize; The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness; and Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain. He has also received the Prince of Asturias Award in Science and Technology, the Kappers Neuroscience Medal, the Beaumont Medal from the American Medical Association, the Nonino Prize, the Reenpaa Prize in Neuroscience, and the Honda Prize. – Bowker Author Biography.

SUMMARY = Antonio Damasio, who is one of the most significant neuroscientists at work today, has written a pathbreaking book investigating the question that has confounded philosophers, psychologists, and neuroscientists for centuries. How is consciousness created?

BOOK DESCRIPTION = Damasio has spent the past 30 years studying and writing about how the brain operates. His work has garnered acclaim for its singular melding of the scientific and the humanistic. In the book, Damasio goes against the long-standing idea that "consciousness" is somehow separate from the body, presenting compelling new scientific evidence that consciousness (what we think of as a mind with a self) is to begin with a biological process created by a living organism.

Besides the three traditional perspectives used to study the mind (the introspective, the behavioral, and the neurological), Damasio introduces an evolutionary perspective that entails a radical change in the way the story of the history of conscious minds is viewed and told. He also advances a radical hypothesis regarding the origins and varieties of feelings, which is central to his framework for the "biological construction of consciousness:" feelings are grounded in a near fusion of body and brain networks, and first emerge from the historically old and humble brain stem rather than from the modern cerebral cortex.

Damasio suggests that the brain's development of a human self becomes a challenge to nature's indifference and opens the way for the appearance of culture. This is a radical break in the course of evolution and the source of a new level of "life regulation." It is called "socio-cultural homeostasis." He leaves no doubt that the blueprint for the work-in-progress he calls socio-cultural homeostasis is the genetically well-established basic homeostasis, the curator of value that has been present in simple life-forms for billions of years.

The book is a groundbreaking journey into the neurobiological foundations of the mind and the self.

PUBLISHERS WEEKLY REVIEW = As he has done previously, USC neuroscientist Damasio (Descartes' Error and other books) explores the process that leads to consciousness. And as he has also done previously, he alternates between some exquisite passages that represent the best popular science has to offer and some technical verbiage that few will be able to follow. He draws meaningful distinctions among points on the continuum from brain to mind, consciousness to self, constantly attempting to understand the evolutionary reasons why each arose and attempting to tie each to an underlying physical reality.

Damasio goes to great lengths to explain that many species, such as social insects, have minds, but humans are distinguished by the "autobiographical self," which adds flexibility and creativity, and has led to the development of culture, a "radical novelty" in natural history. Damasio ends with a speculative chapter on the evolutionary process by which mind developed and then gave rise to self. In the Pleistocene, he suggests, humans developed emotive responses to shapes and sounds that helped lead to the development of the arts. Readers fascinated from both a philosophical and scientific perspective with the question of the relationships among brain, mind, and self will be rewarded for making the effort to follow Damasio's arguments.

green separator
EXCERPT
green separator

PART 1 — Starting Over

When I woke up, we were descending. I had been asleep long enough to miss the announcements about the landing and the weather. I had not been aware of myself or my surroundings. I had been unconscious.

Few things about our biology are as seemingly trivial as this commodity known as consciousness, the phenomenal ability that consists of having a mind equipped with an owner, a protagonist for one’s existence, a self inspecting the world inside and around, an agent seemingly ready for action.

Consciousness is not merely wakefulness. When I woke up, two brief paragraphs ago, I did not look around vacantly, taking in the sights and the sounds as if my awake mind belonged to no one. On the contrary, I knew, almost instantly, with little hesitation if any, without effort, that this was me, sitting on an airplane, my ?ying identity coming home to Los Angeles with a long to-do list before the day would be over, aware of an odd combination of travel fatigue and enthusiasm for what was ahead, curious about the runway we would be landing on, and attentive to the adjustments of engine power that were bringing us to earth. No doubt, being awake was indispensable to this state, but wakefulness was hardly its main feature. What was that main feature? The fact that the myriad contents displayed in my mind, regardless of how vivid or well ordered, connected with me, the proprietor of my mind, through invisible strings that brought those contents together in the forward-moving feast we call self; and, no less important, the fact that the connection was felt. There was a feelingness to the experience of the connected me.

Awakening meant having my temporarily absent mind returned, but with me in it, both property (the mind) and proprietor (me) accounted for. Awakening allowed me to reemerge and survey my mental domains, the sky-wide projection of a magic movie, part documentary and part fiction, otherwise known as the conscious human mind.

We all have free access to consciousness, bubbling so easily and abundantly in our minds that without hesitation or apprehension we let it be turned off every night when we go to sleep and allow it to return every morning when the alarm clock rings, at least 365 times a year, not counting naps. And yet few things about our beings are as remarkable, foundational, and seemingly mysterious as consciousness. Without consciousness — that is, a mind endowed with subjectivity — you would have no way of knowing that you exist, let alone know who you are and what you think. Had subjectivity not begun, even if very modestly at ?rst, in living creatures far simpler than we are, memory and reasoning are not likely to have expanded in the prodigious way they did, and the evolutionary road for language and the elaborate human version of consciousness we now possess would not have been paved. Creativity would not have flourished. There would have been no song, no painting, and no literature. Love would never have been love, just sex. Friendship would have been mere cooperative convenience. Pain would never have become suffering — not a bad thing, come to think of it — but an equivocal advantage given that pleasure would not have become bliss either. Had subjectivity not made its radical appearance, there would have been no knowing and no one to take notice, and consequently there would have been no history of what creatures did through the ages, no culture at all.

Although I have not yet provided a working definition of consciousness, I hope I am leaving no doubt as to what it means not to have consciousness: in the absence of consciousness, the personal view is suspended; we do not know of our existence; and we do not know that anything else exists. If consciousness had not developed in the course of evolution and expanded to its human version, the humanity we are now familiar with, in all its frailty and strength, would never have developed either. One shudders to think that a simple turn not taken might have meant the loss of the biological alternatives that make us truly human. But then, how would we ever have found out that something was missing?

We take consciousness for granted because it is so available, so easy to use, so elegant in its daily disappearing and reappearing acts, and yet, when we think of it, scientists and non-scientists alike, we do puzzle. What is consciousness made of? Mind with a twist, it seems to me, since we cannot be conscious without having a mind to be conscious of. But what is mind made of? Does mind come from the air or from the body? Smart people say it comes from the brain, that it is in the brain, but that is not a satisfactory reply. How does the brain dominate?

The fact that no one sees the minds of others, conscious or not, is especially mysterious. We can observe their bodies and their actions, what they do or say or write, and we can make informed guesses about what they think. But we cannot observe their minds, and only we ourselves can observe ours, from the inside, and through a rather narrow window. The properties of minds, let alone conscious minds, appear to be so radically different from those of visible living matter that thoughtful folk wonder how one process (conscious minds working) meshes with the other process (physical cells living together in aggregates called tissues).

But to say that conscious minds are mysterious — and on the face of it they are — is different from saying that the mystery is insoluble. It is different from saying that we shall never be able to understand how a living organism endowed with a brain develops a conscious mind.

www.antoniodamasio.com

separator
REMEMBER ALWAYS:
You Are Your Adaptable Memory!
separator

Click or Tap Star to:

Antonio Damasio

HUMANIST HUB
OF SECULAR SCIENCE STARS


ALPHABETICAL BRAIN® VOCABULARY

green separator
produced by
Infinite Interactive Ideas®